Google [Emotion, Cognition, and Mental State Representation Salzman] for research, reported in the Science News’ article, Cerebral Delights, which identifies primary neurological links between fear and need. Perhaps science will eventually discover most everything that is discoverable (1).
I have felt for several years that fear stood at the headwaters of all emotions, including those related to need. Additionally, what fear and need mean is less straightforward than often thought. Therefore, before diving into this, I should clarify my sense of these words, especially need. For this, please see What are the roots of thought? (p.602) to review my caveats concerning need and fear.
Learning to speak Thai first deepened my understanding of need. I picked up the Thai language the easy way – living among non-English speaking Thai folks. A particularly striking difference in thinking between my Western upbringing and the Thai culture was in the use and meaning of the word want and need. I was accustomed to think need, want, and desire were different. For instance, I may want that yacht; however, do I really need it? By contrast, Thai’s, at least at the peasant level, don’t perceive a sharp distinction between need, want, and desire. The synonymic nature of these words made sense to me, as did their Buddhist worldview.
Regarding need, want, and desire as virtual synonyms applies to this research. All of these connect deeply to our pursuit of pleasure. Similarly, fear, worry, and insecurity link deeply to our avoidance of pain. Consider these contrasting words: need vs. fear; pleasure vs. pain; attraction vs. repulsion. While we’re at it, why not include love vs. hate; beauty vs. ugly; and good vs. bad. Do you see how all these words correlate?
Need ≈ pleasure ≈ attraction≈ love ≈ beauty≈ good
——- ———– —— ————- —————————
Fear ≈ pain ≈ repulsion ≈ hate ≈ ugly ≈ bad
Now let’s dive in…
I am happy to see some objective scientific evidence linking need with fear since my research is quite literally just the opposite — a thoroughly subjective experience. Mine is a murky research environment, yet in some ways, it can be the only effective way to examine life deeply. After all, before we know it life is over and we return to the great Nothing, whereas science crawls along for the foreseeable future. On the other hand, fooling ourselves is all too easily, so I welcome any supporting evidence. I’ll paste the most pertinent passages below. (For the Science News report, google [Amygdala Science News Feb 26,2011 PDF].)
The amygdala, a part of the brain known for its role in fear, also helps people spot rewards — and go after them.
For years the amygdala has been regarded primarily as the brain’s center for fear. Scores of studies have shown that it is essential both for perceiving fear and expressing it.
In recent years, though, a surge of new research has expanded scientists’ view of the amygdala’s importance. It turns out that the amygdala helps shape behavior in response to all sorts of stimuli, bad and good. It plays a role not only in aversion to fright, but also in pursuit of pleasure.
Studies of the brain’s anatomy reveal good reasons for the amygdala’s power: It is very well connected. In humans and other primates, the amygdala is linked through a complex network of cells to brain regions involved in all five senses. Signals about everything you encounter are passed from the brain’s sensory processing areas directly to the amygdala. And the amygdala shares elaborate communications channels with the prefrontal cortex — the brain’s control center for planning and decision making.
Its strategic location allows the amygdala to act as a spotlight, calling attention to sensory input that is new, exciting and important. In this way, it helps predict the timing and location of potential dangers, helping you dodge many of the things you dread. But those same connections also help you acquire the good things in life, by identifying and assessing rewards such as food, sex and other delights.
Though much more is known about its fear job, researchers are now vigorously gathering evidence about how the amygdala evaluates information and events for their reward potential. Recent studies offer clues to how the amygdala assigns value to rewards and adjusts that value as circumstances change. Other work provides insights into how the amygdala links actions and rewards, suggesting that the amygdala plays a role in goal-directed behavior. Still others are finding out how neural circuits in the highly connected human amygdala work with other brain structures to recognize good things and find ways to get them.
Toward the end of the article the following appears. It reminds us of how fuzzy the view can become the closer you get. The main point here is how important the amygdala is for assigning an emotional value, with the primary focus on pleasure and fear. In other words, we feel a need of pleasure and a fear of pain.
Rudebeck and his group trained monkeys to play a computer game in which they assessed the value of different rewards. The animals were shown two different pictures and allowed to choose between them. One picture brought a large juice reward, and the other brought a much smaller amount of juice. The animals chose the picture associated with the larger reward more than 98 percent of the time.
After turning off the amygdala in some animals, the scientists used single-cell recordings to listen in on brain cell chatter in the orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex. To the team’s surprise, the monkeys still chose the picture with the “best” outcome on pretty much every trial, just as they had done with a working amygdala.
Though the animals continued choosing in the same manner, the scientists found that fewer neurons in the anterior cingulate cortex changed their firing rate in response to an expected reward. When looking at the animals’ emotional responses — as measured by pupil diameter and heart rate — researchers found that monkeys without a working amygdala didn’t react to a reward in the typical way, Rudebeck says. “They seemed to have no idea of what reward was, despite the fact that they could still choose perfectly well.”
The findings, reported at the neuroscience meeting, suggest that the brain uses various mechanisms to calculate how much something is worth. While the amygdala may be important for assigning an emotional value, Rudebeck says, it may not be the “be-all and end-all” in valuing objects.
(1) Of course, the most important thing from a Taoist point of view is nothing, which is beyond the scope of science. With nothing, there is nothing to grasp. Fear and nothing are closely related, so anything science finds about the nature of fear may tell us something about nothing, if we read between the lines anyway. Still, I feel chapter 40 sums it up well…
Great example! The accumulating mountain of empirical evidence shows that we are our biology. This makes our resilient belief in free will all the more striking. Seen from a symptoms point of view, this must only indicate how terrified we are to be one with nature… and just another animal as it were. We desperately desire to control; we fear lacking control. Is this because we are cognitively aware (think) of so many ‘choices’. Our mind’s eye is all over the place: past, present and future.
Now that was interesting. I haven’t been keeping abreast of psychological research since the degree I did a few years back, and I remember the amygdala being introduced as the controller of fear. Have you heard about the patient who had a calcified amygdala? With no social anxieties, she would hug everyone, smiled all the time, and was generally cheerful. Without a full perception of “need/fear”, she must have been quite the taoist, free of desire.
(The flipside of this story is that she fell in love repeatedly and was generally treated badly, since she had no apparent concept of good or bad qualities in a man (for want of a better phrase).)